Showing posts with label Zoo Keepers. Show all posts
Showing posts with label Zoo Keepers. Show all posts

Friday, October 14, 2011

Why Do You Work In A Zoo?



A qualitative examination of work meaning in the zookeeping profession pointed to the centrality of the notion of work as a personal calling. The view of calling expressed by zookeepers, however, was closer in basic structure to the classical conceptualization of the Protestant reformers than it was to more recent formulations.
We used qualitative data from interviews with U.S. zookeepers to develop hypotheses about the implications of this neoclassical conceptualization of calling for the relationship between individuals and their work. We found that a neoclassical calling is both binding and ennobling.
On one hand, zookeepers with a sense of calling strongly identified with and found broader meaning and significance in their work and occupation. On the other hand,
they were more likely to see their work as a moral duty, to sacrifice pay, personal time, and comfort for their work, and to hold their zoo to a higher standard. Results of a survey of zookeepers from 157 different zoos in the U.S. and Canada supported the hypotheses from our emergent theory. These results reveal the ways in which deeply meaningful work can become a double-edged sword.•

Peter's Note: This is not a new article but it is well worth reading. I mean:

Why Do You Work In A Zoo?

***



For regular updated Zoo News, Views, Reviews and Vacancies please visit
 Zoo News Digest
on



Please Visit
The Zoo Professionals Book Store
if you are looking for a book. This is an Amazon affiliate and you will get a good deal

also
 you can subscribe to the largest and longest established zoo related ezine
by clicking
HERE




To advertise in Zoo News Digest please see HERE 



Money for Free
If you can write, you can earn. Write about what you like or what you know about. Build up a passive income which will earn you money daily. No catch, no charge. Click on the link below and you could start earning today


HubPages



Saturday, January 16, 2010

Eclipse Causes Odd Behaviour in Zoo Keepers

Zoo keepers behaved strangely

The annular eclipse on Thursday had no effect on animals at the zoo, even the citizens if the thousands of visitors to the zoo are an indication, but it appears to have affected only the zoo keepers.






The staff of the zoo changed the feeding time of the animals to skirt the eclipse period during which superstition forbids consumption of food. They fed the animals two hours before the normal morning feeding time of 11.30 and one hour after the eclipse was over and done with, at 4.30 p.m.



Neither the eclipse nor the change of the feeding time had any effect on the animals who were absolutely normal. The birds were chirpy, the tiger napped under a tree in his open enclosure, felines paced around in their cages, a chimp danced in his enclosure, and the snakes slithered around lazily as ever.



With many the citizens having given themselves a voluntary holiday, the zoo officials’ day began with calls wanting to know if the zoo was open for the day. As the day wore on, hundreds of families, local as well as tourists visited the zoo.



Asked why the food was served early, Dr C Suresh Kumar, assistant director of the Zoo Hospital said some animal keepers insisted that it was better to feed the animal before the solar eclipse starts which was accepted without questioning. Accordingly, the zoo staff prepared the food as early as possible and served the animals and birds. But as per the animal keeper’s advise the plans to shift a baboon to a new enclosure by tranquilising was postponed to tomorrow (Saturday) because of sentiments.



Contrary to strong belief that the birds and animals behave abnormally during Solar eclipse, the animals and showed no change in behaviour. Dr Suresh Kumar admitted that it is difficult to say whether the animals and birds behave abnormally on a solar eclipse day without conducting a scientific study in this regard. There are more than 1,200 birds and animals of 140 different species and conducting scientific study on all of them is difficult. As the birds are kept in enclosure covered on all three directions there is no scope for them to feel or experience the rays. “I went around the enclosure of almost all the birds and animals and all of them were found to be normal”.



Manager Shivanna, who has 25 years of experience in the zoo echoed the views of Dr Suresh Kumar. But, out of curiosity, animal keepers were asked to closely observe their respective birds and animals and report in case of

Read Full Story

Friday, December 25, 2009

Keeping Elephants can be very Dangerous Work

Risky business of keeping captive animals


Wild at heart — at least in part
 
 

 
Twenty years ago, the most dangerous job in America was tending elephants. Keepers were three times more likely to die on the job than coal miners, who had the second most dangerous occupation. Between 1989 and 1991 alone, six elephant keepers were killed at work, prompting zoo officials across the country to ask three questions: Should they continue with the time-honored, hands-on management of elephants? Should they limit handling to situations in which the elephants were safely restrained? Or should they cease keeper-elephant contact altogether?




None of the options was ideal. The first was clearly hazardous. “Lots of people were getting injured; lots of people were getting killed,” said Jeff Andrews, a veteran elephant keeper at the San Diego Wild Animal Park. The second option was largely impractical: Only a handful of zoos at the time even had the means to effectively restrain an adult elephant, which can weigh up to 12 tons. The third choice wasn’t actually possible: Healthy, happy elephants require daily care and maintenance.



Most zoos in the early 1990s opted to stick with tradition. Keepers continued to work up close and personal with elephants, with little protection beyond their own wits and a hooked stick called an ankus. The method was known as “free contact.”



“Free contact derived from thousands of years of close work with Asian elephants,” said Andrews. “These were working elephants and they were treated a lot like horses. It was believed they needed to be broken, with discipline and dominance established (by the keeper). It wasn’t an official or standardized approach. It was just something that evolved over time, though elephant keepers basically did things the same way.”



Injury and the risk of death were considered part of the job.



“When you’re working with an animal that’s 50 times larger, people are going to get hurt, sometimes accidentally, sometimes not,” said Andrews.



It would take the challenge of caring for another large and potentially lethal mammal to change the ways things were done.



In the wild, orcas or killer whales are not considered to be a threat to humans. In captivity, they can be, though their record of injuring and killing keepers or trainers pales in comparison to elephants. “Whales are big and smart and you can’t make them do anything they don’t want to do,” said Bill Winhall, an assistant curator of mammals at SeaWorld San Diego. “When you’re in the water with them, it’s good to remember that and always pay attention.”



Like zookeepers, the animal handlers at SeaWorld and similar places were keen to find safer ways of dealing with orcas, dolphins, walruses and other big marine mammals. It wasn’t just about training for shows; it was about basic care and maintenance.



“Back in the 1970s, if you just wanted to take a blood sample from a killer whale, you needed to lower the pool, put the whale in a sling and then use 10 to 12 people plus a crane to hoist the animal out of the water for the procedure,” said Gary Priest, who once worked at SeaWorld and is now curator of animal behavior for the San Diego Zoological Society.



At SeaWorld and elsewhere, though, a new concept was bubbling toward the surface. Perhaps an animal’s behavioral training could be used to make routine care easier and safer. Killer whales, for example, were already trained to flop down on poolside platforms during shows, perhaps they could be induced to stay there long enough and quietly enough for minor medical procedures like blood collection. The answer proved to be a resounding yes. Indeed, Priest said it took just seven days to teach the first whale to calmly present its tail fluke for an injection, a process that began with desensitizing pin pricks using car keys.



“Things got much easier and more relaxed after that,” said Priest, who helped bring the approach — dubbed “protected contact” — to the San Diego Zoo and its entirely different menagerie.



Protected contact is now standard operating procedure in most major zoos and places where humans must work closely with potentially dangerous animals, but the transition from free contact did not come easily or fast, either for humans or zoo animals. For one thing, skeptics insisted that the only way to exert control over an elephant was to make sure it knew who was boss.



“Free-contact elephant keepers are a bit like jet fighter pilots,” said Priest. “Neither believes anything bad will happen to them. Both have incredible self-confidence. If you work with elephants, that’s a good thing. You’ve got to have a lot of confidence and always show it because if you don’t, the animals can tell and you’re at greater risk.”



Intelligent creatures like whales and elephants are always testing the status quo, challenging the hierarchy to see who’s in charge, keepers say. Acts of insubordination may be subtle: a slight nudge or a slowness to respond, for example. Or they may be overt, such as a whale pulling a trainer underwater or scraping them along the bottom of the pool. If the keeper or trainer isn’t prepared, quick to act with the appropriate response, an elephant or whale may push things even harder, perhaps catastrophically so.



“These are wild animals and they always will be,” said Winhall. “An animal like a polar bear is an apex predator with a matching temperament. It will sit patiently next to an ice hole for, like, forever just waiting for a seal to pop its head out. That’s something to remember when you’re working with them” or any other creature capable of causing harm.



Happy to learn



In free contact, any sign of disobedience or rebellion in an animal is unacceptable. There can be no mistakes of behavior by either the animal or keeper because there’s almost no safety margin. Protected contact, on the other hand, recognizes that everybody, even whales, elephants and other creatures, have bad days.



“The animals are voluntary participants,” said Priest. “We don’t make them do things they don’t want to do. There aren’t negative consequences.”



It took time for both San Diego zookeepers and elephants originally trained with free-contact methods to learn and adapt to the new system.



“The elephants discovered that if they chose not to comply with a request, we weren’t going to go in and require compliance,” said Priest. “It was almost funny to watch the expressions on their faces when they walked away and we didn’t follow. They were like little kids who thought they had gotten away with something, only they weren’t quite sure what it was.”



In fact, said Andrews at the Wild Animal Park, the elephants probably didn’t know what to think or do. “Free contact-trained elephants are taught not to do anything until they are told to, so they tend not to really think for themselves. It took many years for the older elephants to come around and really trust us.”



In 2003, the Wild Animal Park received seven wild elephants from Swaziland, all 3 or 4 years old and all slated to be killed as part of a herd-reduction program. The Swaziland elephants were the first to be trained entirely using protected contact methods. Critics again scoffed, insisting the elephants would first need to be broken, but Andrews said they adapted to protected contact with surprising alacrity.



“Elephants are excited to learn. You can actually see the animal thinking about what it should do,” Andrews said. “Maybe they do it wrong a few times, but then they’ll do it exactly right, you respond and their eyes light up as if they’re thinking, ‘Oh, so that’s what he wants.’ ”



The basic learning process is called operant conditioning, a method in which voluntary responses are modified to achieve a specific behavior or outcome, usually through rewards of food or praise. The training is incremental, tiny steps called “successive approximations” that are small movements building upon each other until a full-fledged behavior is accomplished. The elephants at the San Diego Zoo and Wild Animal Park are trained to do 32 behaviors, everything from opening their mouth to lifting their feet to laying down. Each behavior makes it possible for keepers to conduct the various examinations or procedures necessary for their health without actually stepping inside the elephant’s enclosure.



The Wild Animal Park was the first zoo facility in the country to modify its elephant barrier system to accommodate protected contact methods. The new Elephant Odyssey exhibit at the San Diego Zoo is the first elephant enclosure designed strictly for protected contact. With modifications, the method appears to work for most animals.



“The nice thing about cats is they’re very food-motivated, very into meat,” said Autumn Nelson, a big cat curator at the zoo, where keepers use meat (or treat items like blood, bone marrow and chicken broth) to teach cats elemental behaviors like walking into a crate, opening their mouth or sticking out a paw for inspection when they see a particular hand gesture or hear a specific word or sound, like a whistle or click.



There are differences between species, of course. Lions respond well to training, but they’re very social animals and sometimes pay more attention to each other than to the trainer. Tigers, on the other hand, are solitary animals. They bond more slowly with zoo staff, said Nelson, but “you don’t have other animals interfering with the training either.”



Working with apes is a bit like working with people, said Greg Vicino, an animal care supervisor at the zoo. If keepers come to work grumpy or frustrated, the

>>>READ FULL ARTICLE>>>

Thursday, December 24, 2009

Zoo Keeper Gored By Enraged Buffalo



On This Day In History

on this day in history: 1911: zoo keeper gored by enraged buffalo. from the 12/22/11 farmington observer.

Posted in rememberance of all the Zoo Keepers who have lost their lives or have been severely injured in the course of their duty during 2009. Let us make 2010 a Health and Safety year. Think before you act.


Thursday, November 26, 2009

Sorry Kuwait Zoo

In the editorial to my last Zoo News Digest I made a few remarks about Kuwait Zoo which were based on wrongly interpreted assumptions. How do I know?

A friend and colleague of mine has just returned from a visit to Kuwait Zoo. If it were just anybody informing me I may not believe what they had to say. He tells me that there are actually Kuwaiti keepers and supervisors within Kuwait Zoo and that they appear dedicated to their work. This includes the zoo director, Farid a Mulla Ahmad who is doing an extremely good job of work.

The newspaper article which began my suppositions had been put in the paper to draw peoples attention to their bad behaviour in the hope that it may lessen during the Eid Holidays. I do hope that it does.

Some of the things I recollect zoo visitors doing to animals and giving to animals are enough to bring a shiver down my spine and most of these would be from the UK!

So, Sorry Kuwait Zoo. My friend says you are dedicated and doing a good job. Thank you and keep up the good work. Shukran.